Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.
- View all journals
- Explore content
- About the journal
- Publish with us
- Sign up for alerts
- News Feature
- Published: 14 March 2012
The split brain: A tale of two halves
- David Wolman 1
Nature volume 483 , pages 260–263 ( 2012 ) Cite this article
98k Accesses
30 Citations
612 Altmetric
Metrics details
- Medical research
- Neurosurgery
Since the 1960s, researchers have been scrutinizing a handful of patients who underwent a radical kind of brain surgery. The cohort has been a boon to neuroscience — but soon it will be gone.
In the first months after her surgery, shopping for groceries was infuriating. Standing in the supermarket aisle, Vicki would look at an item on the shelf and know that she wanted to place it in her trolley — but she couldn't. “I'd reach with my right for the thing I wanted, but the left would come in and they'd kind of fight,” she says. “Almost like repelling magnets.” Picking out food for the week was a two-, sometimes three-hour ordeal. Getting dressed posed a similar challenge: Vicki couldn't reconcile what she wanted to put on with what her hands were doing. Sometimes she ended up wearing three outfits at once. “I'd have to dump all the clothes on the bed, catch my breath and start again.”
In one crucial way, however, Vicki was better than her pre-surgery self. She was no longer racked by epileptic seizures that were so severe they had made her life close to unbearable. She once collapsed onto the bar of an old-fashioned oven, burning and scarring her back. “I really just couldn't function,” she says. When, in 1978, her neurologist told her about a radical but dangerous surgery that might help, she barely hesitated. If the worst were to happen, she knew that her parents would take care of her young daughter. “But of course I worried,” she says. “When you get your brain split, it doesn't grow back together.”
In June 1979, in a procedure that lasted nearly 10 hours, doctors created a firebreak to contain Vicki's seizures by slicing through her corpus callosum, the bundle of neuronal fibres connecting the two sides of her brain. This drastic procedure, called a corpus callosotomy, disconnects the two sides of the neocortex, the home of language, conscious thought and movement control. Vicki's supermarket predicament was the consequence of a brain that behaved in some ways as if it were two separate minds.
After about a year, Vicki's difficulties abated. “I could get things together,” she says. For the most part she was herself: slicing vegetables, tying her shoe laces, playing cards, even waterskiing.
But what Vicki could never have known was that her surgery would turn her into an accidental superstar of neuroscience. She is one of fewer than a dozen 'split-brain' patients, whose brains and behaviours have been subject to countless hours of experiments, hundreds of scientific papers, and references in just about every psychology textbook of the past generation. And now their numbers are dwindling.
Through studies of this group, neuroscientists now know that the healthy brain can look like two markedly different machines, cabled together and exchanging a torrent of data. But when the primary cable is severed, information — a word, an object, a picture — presented to one hemisphere goes unnoticed in the other. Michael Gazzaniga, a cognitive neuroscientist at the University of California, Santa Barbara, and the godfather of modern split-brain science, says that even after working with these patients for five decades, he still finds it thrilling to observe the disconnection effects first-hand. “You see a split-brain patient just doing a standard thing — you show him an image and he can't say what it is. But he can pull that same object out of a grab-bag,” Gazzaniga says. “Your heart just races!”
Nature Podcast
Michael Gazzaniga reflects on five decades of split-brain research
Work with the patients has teased out differences between the two hemispheres, revealing, for instance, that the left side usually leads the way for speech and language computation, and the right specializes in visual-spatial processing and facial recognition. “The split work really showed that the two hemispheres are both very competent at most things, but provide us with two different snapshots of the world,” says Richard Ivry, director of the Institute of Cognitive and Brain Sciences at the University of California, Berkeley. The idea of dichotomous consciousness captivated the public, and was greatly exaggerated in the notion of the 'creative right brain'. But further testing with split-brain patients gave a more-nuanced picture. The brain isn't like a computer, with specific sections of hardware charged with specific tasks. It's more like a network of computers connected by very big, busy broadband cables. The connectivity between active brain regions is turning out to be just as important, if not more so, than the operation of the distinct parts. “With split-brain patients, you can see the impact of disconnecting a huge portion of that network, but without damage to any particular modules,” says Michael Miller, a psychologist at the University of California, Santa Barbara.
David Roberts, head of neurosurgery at Dartmouth-Hitchcock Medical Center in Lebanon, New Hampshire, sees an important lesson in split-brain research. He operated on some of the cohort members, and has worked closely with Gazzaniga. “In medical school, and science in general, there is so much emphasis on large numbers, labs, diagnostics and statistical significance,” Roberts says — all crucial when, say, evaluating a new drug. But the split-brain cohort brought home to him how much can be gleaned from a single case. “I came to learn that one individual, studied well, and thoughtfully, might enable you to draw conclusions that apply to the entire human species,” he says.
Today, the split-brain patients are getting on in years; a few have died, one has had a stroke and age in general has made them all less fit for what can be taxing research sessions of sitting, staring and concentrating. The surgery, already quite rare, has been replaced by drug treatments and less drastic surgical procedures. Meanwhile, imaging technologies have become the preferred way to look at brain function, as scientists can simply watch which areas of the brain are active during a task.
But to Miller, Ivry, Gazzaniga and others, split-brain patients remain an invaluable resource. Imaging tools can confirm, for example, that the left hemisphere is more active than the right when processing language. But this is dramatically embodied in a split-brain patient, who may not be able to read aloud a word such as 'pan' when it's presented to the right hemisphere, but can point to the appropriate drawing. “That gives you a sense of the right hemisphere's ability to read, even if it can't access the motor system to produce speech,” Ivry says. “Imaging is very good for telling you where something happens,” he adds, “whereas patient work can tell you how something happens.”
A cable, cut
Severing the corpus callosum was first used as a treatment for severe epilepsy in the 1940s, on a group of 26 people in Rochester, New York. The aim was to limit the electrical storm of the seizure to one side of the brain. At first, it didn't seem to work. But in 1962, one patient showed significant improvement. Although the procedure never became a favoured treatment strategy — it's invasive, risky, and drugs can ease symptoms in many people — in the decades since it nevertheless became a technique of last resort for treating intractable epilepsy.
To Roger Sperry, then a neurobiologist and neuropsychologist at the California Institute of Technology, and Gazzaniga, a graduate student in Sperry's lab, split-brain patients presented a unique opportunity to explore the lateralized nature of the human brain. At the time, opinion on the matter was itself divided. Researchers who studied the first split-brain patients in the 1940s had concluded that the separation didn't noticeably affect thought or behaviour. (Gazzaniga and others suspect that these early sections were incomplete, which might also explain why they didn't help the seizures.) Conversely, studies conducted by Sperry and colleagues in the 1950s revealed greatly altered brain function in animals that had undergone callosal sections. Sperry and Gazzaniga became obsessed with this inconsistency, and saw in the split-brain patients a way to find answers.
The duo's first patient was a man known as W. J., a former Second World War paratrooper who had started having seizures after a German soldier clocked him in the head with the butt of a rifle. In 1962, after W.J.'s operation, Gazzaniga ran an experiment in which he asked W.J. to press a button whenever he saw an image. Researchers would then flash images of letters, light bursts and other stimuli to his left or right field of view. Because the left field of view is processed by the right hemisphere and vice versa, flashing images quickly to one side or the other delivers the information solely to the intended hemisphere (see 'Of two minds').
For stimuli delivered to the left hemisphere, W.J. showed no hang-ups; he simply pressed the button and told the scientists what he saw. With the right hemisphere, W.J. said he saw nothing, yet his left hand kept pressing the button every time an image appeared. “The left and right didn't know what the other was doing,” says Gazzaniga. It was a paradigm-blasting discovery showing that the brain is more divided than anyone had predicted 1 .
Suddenly, the race was on to delve into the world of lateralized function. But finding more patients to study proved difficult. Gazzaniga estimates that at least 100 patients, and possibly many more, received a corpus callosotomy. But individuals considered for the operation tend to have other significant developmental or cognitive problems; only a few have super-clean cuts and are neurologically healthy enough to be useful to researchers. For a while, Sperry, Gazzaniga and their colleagues didn't know if there was ever going to be anyone else like W.J..
But after contacting neurosurgeons, partnering with epilepsy centres and assessing many potential patients, they were able to identify a few suitable people in California, then a cluster from the eastern part of the United States, including Vicki. Through the 1970s and the early 1980s, split-brain research expanded, and neuroscientists became particularly interested in the capabilities of the right hemisphere — the one conventionally believed to be incapable of processing language and producing speech.
Gazzaniga can tick through the names of his “endlessly patient patients” with the ease of a proud grandparent doing a roll call of grandchildren — W.J., A.A., R.Y., L.B., N.G.. For medical confidentiality, they are known in the literature by initials only. (Vicki agreed to be identified in this article, provided that her last name and hometown were not published.)
On stage last May, delivering a keynote address at the Society of Neurological Surgeons' annual meeting in Portland, Oregon, Gazzaniga showed a few grainy film clips from a 1976 experiment with patient P.S., who was only 13 or 14 at the time. The scientists wanted to see his response if only his right hemisphere saw written words.
In Gazzaniga's video, the boy is asked: who is your favourite girlfriend, with the word girlfriend flashed only to the right hemisphere. As predicted, the boy can't respond verbally. He shrugs and shakes his head, indicating that he doesn't see any word, as had been the case with W.J.. But then he giggles. It's one of those tell-tale teen giggles — a soundtrack to a blush. His right hemisphere has seen the message, but the verbal left-hemisphere remains unaware. Then, using his left hand, the boy slowly selects three Scrabble tiles from the assortment in front of him. He lines them up to spell L-I-Z: the name, we can safely assume, of the cute girl in his class. “That told us that he was capable of language comprehension in the right hemisphere,” Gazzaniga later told me. “He was one of the first confirmation cases that you could get bilateral language — he could answer queries using language from either side.”
The implications of these early observations were “huge”, says Miller. They showed that “the right hemisphere is experiencing its own aspect of the world that it can no longer express, except through gestures and control of the left hand”. A few years later, the researchers found that Vicki also had a right-hemisphere capacity for speech 2 . Full callosotomy, it turned out, resulted in some universal disconnections, but also affected individuals very differently.
In 1981, Sperry was awarded a share of the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine for the split-brain discoveries. (“He deserved it,” Gazzaniga says.) Sperry died in 1994, but by that point, Gazzaniga was leading the charge. By the turn of the century, he and other split-brain investigators had turned their attention to another mystery: despite the dramatic effects of callosotomy, W.J. and later patients never reported feeling anything less than whole. As Gazzaniga wrote many times: the hemispheres didn't miss each other.
Gazzaniga developed what he calls the interpreter theory to explain why people — including split-brain patients — have a unified sense of self and mental life 3 . It grew out of tasks in which he asked a split-brain person to explain in words, which uses the left hemisphere, an action that had been directed to and carried out only by the right one. “The left hemisphere made up a post hoc answer that fit the situation.” In one of Gazzaniga's favourite examples, he flashed the word 'smile' to a patient's right hemisphere and the word 'face' to the left hemisphere, and asked the patient to draw what he'd seen. “His right hand drew a smiling face,” Gazzaniga recalled. “'Why did you do that?' I asked. He said, 'What do you want, a sad face? Who wants a sad face around?'.” The left-brain interpreter, Gazzaniga says, is what everyone uses to seek explanations for events, triage the barrage of incoming information and construct narratives that help to make sense of the world.
The split-brain studies constitute “an incredible body of work”, said Robert Breeze, a neurosurgeon at the University of Colorado Hospital in Aurora, after listening to Gazzaniga's lecture last year. But Breeze, like many other neuroscientists, sees split-brain research as outdated. “Now we have technologies that enable us to see these things” — tools such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) that show the whereabouts of brain function in great detail.
Miller, however, disagrees. “These kinds of patients can tell us things that fMRI can never tell us,” he says.
Subject of interest
Seated at a small, oval dining-room table, Vicki faces a laptop propped up on a stand, and a console with a few large red and green buttons. David Turk, a psychologist at the University of Aberdeen, UK, has flown in for the week to run a series of experiments.
Vicki's grey-white hair is pulled back in a ponytail. She wears simple white sneakers and, despite the autumn chill, shorts. She doesn't want to get too warm: when that happens she can get drowsy and lose focus, which can wreck a whole day of research.
During a break, Vicki fetches an old photo album. In one picture, taken soon after her surgery, she is sitting up in the hospital bed. Her hair is starting to grow back as black stubble and she and her daughter have wide smiles. Another page of the album has a slightly faded printout of a 1981 paper from The Journal of Neuroscience glued into it: the first published report involving data gleaned from Vicki, in which researchers describe how she, like P.S., had some capacity for language in her right hemisphere 4 .
I have a hard time saying it's all over.
When pressed to share the most difficult aspect of her life in science, the perpetually upbeat Vicki says that it would have to be an apparatus called the dual Purkinje eye tracker. This medieval-looking device requires the wearer to bite down on a bar to help keep the head still so that researchers can present an image to just the left or right field of view. It is quite possible that Vicki has spent more of her waking hours biting down on one of those bars than anyone else on the planet.
Soon, it is time to get back to work. Turk uses some two-sided tape to affix a pair of three-dimensional glasses onto the front of Vicki's thin, gold-rimmed bifocals. The experiment he is running aims to separate the role of the corpus callosum in visual processing from that of deeper, 'subcortical' connections unaffected by the callosotomy. Focusing on the centre of the screen, Vicki is told to watch as the picture slowly switches between a house and different faces — and to press the button every time she sees the image change. Adjusting her seat, she looks down the bridge of her nose at the screen and tells Turk that she's ready to begin.
Deep connections
Other researchers are studying the role of subcortical communication in the coordinated movements of the hands. Split-brain patients have little difficulty with 'bimanual' tasks, and Vicki and at least one other patient are able to drive a car. In 2000, a team led by Liz Franz at the University of Otago in New Zealand asked split-brain patients to carry out both familiar and new bimanual tasks. A patient who was an experienced fisherman, they found, could pantomime tying a fishing line, but not the unfamiliar task of threading a needle. Franz concluded that well-practised bimanual skills are coordinated at the subcortical level, so split-brain people are able to smoothly choreograph both hands 5 .
Miller and Gazzaniga have also started to study the right hemisphere's role in moral reasoning. It is the kind of higher-level function for which the left hemisphere was assumed to be king. But in the past few years, imaging studies have shown that the right hemisphere is heavily involved in the processing of others' emotions, intentions and beliefs — what many scientists have come to understand as the 'theory of mind' 6 . To Miller, the field of enquiry perfectly illustrates the value of split-brain studies because answers can't be found by way of imaging tools alone.
In work that began in 2009, the researchers presented two split-brain patients with a series of stories, each of which involved either accidental or intentional harm. The aim was to find out whether the patients felt that someone who intends to poison his boss but fails because he mistakes sugar for rat poison, is on equal moral ground with someone who accidentally kills his boss by mistaking rat poison for sugar 7 . (Most people conclude that the former is more morally reprehensible.) The researchers read the stories aloud, which meant that the input was directed to the left hemisphere, and asked for verbal responses, so that the left hemisphere, guided by the interpreter mechanism, would also create and deliver the response. So could the split-brain patients make a conventional moral judgement using just that side of the brain?
No. The patients reasoned that both scenarios were morally equal. The results suggest that both sides of the cortex are necessary for this type of reasoning task.
But this finding presents an additional puzzle, because relatives and friends of split-brain patients do not notice unusual reasoning or theory-of-mind deficits. Miller's team speculates that, in everyday life, other reasoning mechanisms may compensate for disconnection effects that are exposed in the lab. It's an idea that he plans to test in the future.
As the opportunities for split-brain research dwindle, Gazzaniga is busy trying to digitize the archive of recordings of tests with cohort members, some of which date back more than 50 years. “Each scene is so easy to remember for me, and so moving,” he says. “We were observing so many astonishing things, and others should have the same opportunity through these videos.” Perhaps, he says, other researchers will even uncover something new.
Other split-brain patients may become available — there is a small cluster in Italy, for instance. But with competition from imaging research and many of the biggest discoveries about the split brain behind him, Gazzaniga admits that the glory days of this field of science are probably gone. “It is winding down in terms of patients commonly tested.” Still, he adds: “I have a hard time saying it's all over.”
And maybe it's not — as long as there are scientists pushing to tackle new questions about lateralized brain function, connectivity and communication, and as long as Vicki and her fellow cohort members are still around and still willing participants in science. Her involvement over the years, Vicki says, was never really about her. “It was always about getting information from me that might help others.”
Gazzaniga, M. S., Bogen, J. E. & Sperry, R. W. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 48 , 1765–1769 (1962).
Article ADS CAS Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. Brain 123 , 1293–1326 (2000).
Article Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. Science 245 , 947–952 (1989).
Sidtis, J. J., Volpe, B. T., Wilson, D. H., Rayport, M. & Gazzaniga, M. S. J. Neurosci. 1 , 323–331 (1981).
Article CAS Google Scholar
Franz, E. A., Waldie, K. E. & Smith, M. J. Psychol. Sci. 11 , 82–85 (2000).
Young, L. & Saxe, R. NeuroImage 40 , 1912–1920 (2008).
Miller, M. B. et al. Neuropsychologia 48 , 2215–2220 (2010).
Download references
Author information
Authors and affiliations.
David Wolman is a freelance writer based in Portland, Oregon, and the author, most recently, of The End of Money.,
David Wolman
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
Related video
Split-brain experiments Michael Gazzaniga and split-brain patient J.W. in experiments shot in the 1990s showing some of the lateralized nature of brain function.
Split-brain work in the 1970s A video featuring Michael Gazzaniga and early split-brain experiments in animals and people.
Related links
Related links in nature research.
Computer modelling: Brain in a box 2012-Feb-22
Neuroscience: The connected self 2012-Feb-01
Neuroscience vs philosophy: Taking aim at free will 2011-Oct-01
Dissecting the right brain 2005-Jul-13
Related external links
Michael Gazzaniga
Rights and permissions
Reprints and permissions
About this article
Cite this article.
Wolman, D. The split brain: A tale of two halves. Nature 483 , 260–263 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1038/483260a
Download citation
Published : 14 March 2012
Issue Date : 15 March 2012
DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/483260a
Share this article
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.
Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
This article is cited by
Visuomotor anomalies in achiasmatic mice expressing a transfer-defective vax1 mutant.
- Kwang Wook Min
- Jin Woo Kim
Experimental & Molecular Medicine (2023)
Complex sequential understanding through the awareness of spatial and temporal concepts
Nature Machine Intelligence (2020)
Split-Brain: What We Know Now and Why This is Important for Understanding Consciousness
- Edward H. F. de Haan
- Paul M. Corballis
Neuropsychology Review (2020)
Oxytocin is implicated in social memory deficits induced by early sensory deprivation in mice
- Jin-Bao Zhang
- Yu-Qiang Ding
Molecular Brain (2016)
Sensory input is required for callosal axon targeting in the somatosensory cortex
- Ning-Ning Song
Molecular Brain (2013)
Quick links
- Explore articles by subject
- Guide to authors
- Editorial policies
Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.
- General Categories
- Mental Health
- IQ and Intelligence
- Bipolar Disorder
Split Brain Experiments: Unveiling the Mysteries of the Divided Mind
A serendipitous accident in the operating room forever changed our understanding of the human mind, as a young epileptic patient emerged from surgery with a brain divided and a consciousness split in two. This unexpected outcome sparked a revolution in neuroscience, leading to decades of groundbreaking research that would challenge our fundamental beliefs about the nature of consciousness, perception, and the very essence of what makes us human.
Imagine, for a moment, the sheer audacity of slicing through the bundle of nerves connecting the two halves of the brain. It sounds like something out of a sci-fi novel, doesn’t it? Yet, this is precisely what happened in the mid-20th century, giving birth to the fascinating field of split brain research. These experiments, which involved severing the corpus callosum – the superhighway of nerve fibers connecting the brain’s hemispheres – opened up a Pandora’s box of questions about how our minds function.
But before we dive headfirst into this rabbit hole of neural wonders, let’s take a step back and consider what we mean by a “split brain.” In essence, it’s exactly what it sounds like – a brain that’s been surgically divided, with the connection between the left and right hemispheres severed. This procedure, initially developed as a last-resort treatment for severe epilepsy, inadvertently became a window into the inner workings of the mind.
The Anatomy of a Split Brain: A Tale of Two Hemispheres
To truly appreciate the significance of split brain research, we need to understand the basic architecture of our noggins. Picture your brain as a walnut – two distinct halves, each with its own unique features and functions, but connected at the core. These halves, known as hemispheres, are responsible for different aspects of our cognitive processes.
The left hemisphere, often dubbed the “logical” side, typically handles language processing, analytical thinking, and sequential reasoning. It’s the part that helps you solve math problems and craft witty comebacks. The right hemisphere, on the other hand, is your creative powerhouse. It’s responsible for spatial awareness, emotional processing, and holistic thinking. It’s what allows you to appreciate a beautiful sunset or understand the punchline of a joke.
Now, here’s where things get interesting. These two hemispheres don’t operate in isolation. They’re constantly chatting with each other, sharing information, and coordinating their efforts. The corpus callosum, a thick bundle of nerve fibers, acts as the brain’s very own information superhighway, allowing for rapid communication between the hemispheres.
But what happens when you cut this superhighway? That’s exactly what split brain problem research set out to discover. By severing the corpus callosum, scientists effectively created two separate “mini-brains” within a single skull. The consequences of this radical procedure were nothing short of mind-blowing.
Pioneering Split Brain Research: A Journey into the Divided Mind
Enter Roger Sperry, a neuropsychologist with a penchant for asking big questions. In the 1960s, Sperry, along with his student Michael Gazzaniga, embarked on a series of experiments that would revolutionize our understanding of the brain. Their subjects? Patients who had undergone corpus callosotomy – the surgical severing of the corpus callosum – as a treatment for severe epilepsy.
Sperry and Gazzaniga’s experiments were deceptively simple, yet profoundly revealing. They presented visual stimuli to one hemisphere of the brain at a time and observed how the patients responded. What they found was nothing short of astonishing.
When an image was shown to the right visual field (processed by the left hemisphere), patients could easily describe what they saw. But when the same image was presented to the left visual field (processed by the right hemisphere), something bizarre happened. Patients claimed they saw nothing, yet they could still point to or draw the object with their left hand (controlled by the right hemisphere).
It was as if the two hemispheres were operating independently, each with its own perceptions and abilities. The left hemisphere, responsible for language, could verbalize what it saw. The right hemisphere, while aware of the image, couldn’t communicate this information verbally.
These findings sent shockwaves through the scientific community. They suggested that consciousness itself might not be a unified phenomenon, but rather a collaboration between two distinct systems within the brain. It was a revelation that would earn Sperry the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine in 1981.
The Methodology of Split Brain Experiments: Peering into the Divided Mind
So, how exactly do scientists study a split brain? The methodology is as fascinating as the results themselves. Visual field experiments, like those conducted by Sperry and Gazzaniga, form the cornerstone of split brain research. By carefully controlling what each hemisphere sees, researchers can tease apart the unique capabilities of each side of the brain.
But vision isn’t the only sense under scrutiny. Tactile and auditory tests have also played a crucial role in unraveling the mysteries of the split brain. For instance, researchers might place an object in a patient’s left hand (controlled by the right hemisphere) and ask them to name it. Surprisingly, split brain patients often struggle with this task, as the language centers in the left hemisphere don’t have access to the tactile information from the right hemisphere.
Language and cognition assessments have been particularly revealing. In some split brain patients, the right hemisphere has shown a limited ability to understand simple language, while the left hemisphere retains more advanced linguistic capabilities. This has led to some truly mind-bending scenarios where a patient’s left hand (controlled by the right hemisphere) might act in ways that contradict their verbal statements.
Of course, studying split brain patients comes with its own set of challenges. These individuals are rare, and the ethical considerations of conducting research on people who have undergone such a significant medical procedure are complex. Moreover, the brain’s remarkable plasticity means that over time, split brain patients often develop compensatory strategies that can mask the effects of the corpus callosotomy.
Key Findings from Split Brain Research: A Tale of Two Minds
So, what have we learned from peering into these divided minds? Perhaps the most striking finding is the extent of hemispheric specialization. While we’ve long known that the brain’s hemispheres have different strengths, split brain research has revealed just how profound these differences can be.
For instance, the left hemisphere typically excels at tasks involving language, logic, and sequential processing. It’s the part of your brain that helps you follow a recipe or solve a crossword puzzle. The right hemisphere, on the other hand, shines when it comes to spatial tasks, emotional processing, and holistic thinking. It’s what allows you to recognize faces, appreciate music, or navigate through a new city.
But it’s not just about specialization. Split brain research has also shed light on how information is processed in the brain. In split brain patients, each hemisphere processes information independently, often leading to some truly bizarre scenarios. For example, a patient might be shown the word “key” in their left visual field (processed by the right hemisphere). When asked what they saw, they might say “nothing.” But if asked to choose a related object from a group of items with their left hand, they would likely pick out a key – all without being consciously aware of why they made that choice.
Perhaps one of the most intriguing aspects of split brain research is what it reveals about language and communication. In most people, language is primarily processed in the left hemisphere. But in split brain patients, the right hemisphere often demonstrates a limited ability to understand simple language, even if it can’t produce speech. This has led to some fascinating experiments where researchers can essentially “talk” to each hemisphere separately.
The implications of split brain research for our understanding of consciousness and self-awareness are profound. If our brain can be divided into two seemingly independent systems, what does this mean for our sense of self? Are we truly one unified consciousness, or are we, in some sense, two minds inhabiting one body?
Applications and Implications: Beyond the Laboratory
While split brain experiments might seem like the stuff of science fiction, their impact extends far beyond the laboratory. The insights gained from this research have had profound implications for neurosurgery, our understanding of neurological disorders, and even our conception of consciousness itself.
In the realm of neurosurgery, split brain research has led to advancements in surgical techniques for treating epilepsy and other neurological conditions. By understanding how the brain can adapt to the loss of inter-hemispheric communication, surgeons can make more informed decisions about the risks and benefits of corpus callosotomy and other brain surgeries.
Split brain research has also provided valuable insights into various neurological disorders. For instance, studies on split brain patients have shed light on the neural mechanisms underlying conditions like dissociation , where individuals experience a disconnection between their thoughts, memories, and sense of identity. This research has paved the way for more effective treatments and interventions for a range of neurological and psychiatric conditions.
One of the most exciting implications of split brain research is what it reveals about brain plasticity and recovery. The fact that split brain patients can often adapt and function relatively normally in everyday life is a testament to the brain’s remarkable ability to rewire itself. This has important implications for rehabilitation after brain injury and for our understanding of how the brain changes and adapts throughout our lives.
Of course, split brain research also raises some thorny ethical questions. If consciousness can be divided, what does this mean for our concepts of personal identity and free will? How should we approach medical decision-making for individuals with split brains? These are complex issues that continue to be debated in neuroscience, philosophy, and bioethics.
The Enduring Legacy of Split Brain Research
As we reflect on the journey of split brain research, from those first serendipitous discoveries to the cutting-edge studies of today, it’s clear that this field has fundamentally transformed our understanding of the brain and mind.
The key findings from split brain experiments have challenged our notions of unified consciousness, revealed the extent of hemispheric specialization, and provided invaluable insights into brain function and plasticity. They’ve shown us that our minds are far more complex and adaptable than we ever imagined.
But the story of split brain research is far from over. Ongoing studies continue to push the boundaries of our knowledge, leveraging advanced neuroimaging techniques and sophisticated experimental designs to probe ever deeper into the mysteries of the divided mind.
For instance, researchers are now exploring how hippocampus brain slice studies can complement split brain research, providing insights into memory formation and retrieval in divided brains. Others are investigating the potential existence of a third hemisphere of the brain , a concept that challenges our traditional two-hemisphere model and could revolutionize our understanding of brain architecture.
The future directions of split brain research are as exciting as they are diverse. From exploring the neural basis of consciousness to developing new treatments for neurological disorders, the potential applications of this research are vast. Some scientists are even investigating how insights from split brain studies might inform the development of artificial intelligence systems, potentially leading to more human-like AI architectures.
As we continue to unravel the mysteries of the split brain, we’re not just learning about a rare condition or an unusual surgical procedure. We’re gaining profound insights into the very nature of human consciousness, cognition, and identity. The split brain experiments have shown us that our minds are far more complex, adaptable, and mysterious than we ever imagined.
In the end, perhaps the most valuable lesson from split brain research is one of humility. It reminds us that despite all our advances, there’s still so much we don’t understand about our own minds. As we peer into the divided brain, we’re not just seeing two hemispheres – we’re catching a glimpse of the vast, uncharted territories of human consciousness that still lie before us.
So, the next time you find yourself of two minds about something, remember – you might be more right than you know!
References:
1. Gazzaniga, M. S. (2005). Forty-five years of split-brain research and still going strong. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(8), 653-659.
2. Sperry, R. W. (1968). Hemisphere deconnection and unity in conscious awareness. American Psychologist, 23(10), 723-733.
3. Wolman, D. (2012). A tale of two halves. Nature, 483(7389), 260-263.
4. Gazzaniga, M. S. (2014). The split-brain: Rooting consciousness in biology. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(51), 18093-18094.
5. Corballis, M. C. (2014). Left brain, right brain: facts and fantasies. PLoS biology, 12(1), e1001767.
6. Zaidel, E. (1994). Interhemispheric transfer in the split brain: Long-term status following complete cerebral commissurotomy. In Human laterality (pp. 491-532). Academic Press.
7. Gazzaniga, M. S., Bogen, J. E., & Sperry, R. W. (1962). Some functional effects of sectioning the cerebral commissures in man. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 48(10), 1765-1769.
8. Turk, D. J., Heatherton, T. F., Kelley, W. M., Funnell, M. G., Gazzaniga, M. S., & Macrae, C. N. (2002). Mike or me? Self-recognition in a split-brain patient. Nature Neuroscience, 5(9), 841-842.
9. Gazzaniga, M. S. (2000). Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: Does the corpus callosum enable the human condition?. Brain, 123(7), 1293-1326.
10. Wolman, D. (2012). The split brain: A tale of two halves. Nature, 483(7389), 260-263.
Was this article helpful?
Would you like to add any comments (optional), leave a reply cancel reply.
Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *
Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.
Post Comment
IMAGES
VIDEO